skip to main content
10.1145/3347451.3356730acmconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication PagesmmConference Proceedingsconference-collections
research-article

Metacognitive Judgments in Searching as Learning (SAL) Tasks: Insights on (Mis-) Calibration, Multimedia Usage, and Confidence

Authors Info & Claims
Published:15 October 2019Publication History

ABSTRACT

Metacognitive self-assessments of one's learning performance (calibration) are important elements of Searching as Learning (SAL) tasks. In this SAL study, N = 115 participants were asked to learn for up to 30 minutes about the formation of thunderstorms and lightning by using any suitable internet resources (including multimedia resources). Participants rated their performance in comparison to other participants (placement), estimated the percentage of correct answers (estimation), and indicated their confidence in the correctness of their answers (confidence) in a multiple-choice knowledge test that was filled in one week before (T1) and directly after (T2) the learning phase. Participants furthermore rated the 'familiarity' of terms that do or do not exist in the context of meteorology (overclaiming). Learners tended to underestimate their performance at T1 and there were indicators of a potential Dunning-Kruger effect. Overall, placement and estimation ratings tended to be more accurate at T2. Surprisingly, confidence ratings increased approximately equally for correct as well as incorrect answers. A propensity for overclaiming was positively correlated with most confidence measures and the amount of time learners spent on YouTube was correlated to lower confidence scores. Implications for the design of SAL tasks and SAL studies are discussed.

References

  1. Pertti Vakkari. 2016. Searching as learning: A systematization based on literature. Journal of Information Science, 42(1), 7--18. http://doi.org/10.1177/0165551515615833Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  2. Ran Yu, Ujwal Gadiraju, Peter Holtz, Markus Rokicki, Philipp Kemkes, and Stefan Dietze. 2018. (June) Predicting user knowledge gain in informational search sessions. In The 41st International ACM SIGIR Conference on Research & Development in Information Retrieval (pp. 75--84). ACM. http://doi.org/10.1145/3209978.3210064Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  3. Ujwal Gadiraju, Ran Yu, Stefan Dietze, and Peter Holtz. 2018. (March). Analyzing knowledge gain of users in informational search sessions on the web. In Proceedings of the 2018 Conference on Human Information Interaction & Retrieval (pp. 2--11). ACM. http://doi.org/10.1145/3176349.3176381Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  4. Nilavra Bhattacharya, and Jacek Gwizdka, 2019 (March). Measuring Learning During Search: Differences in Interactions, Eye-Gaze, and Semantic Similarity to Expert Knowledge. In Proceedings of the 2019 Conference on Human Information Interaction and Retrieval (pp. 63--71). http://doi.org/10.1145/3295750.3298926Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  5. Anett Hoppe, Peter Holtz, Yvonne Kammerer, Ran Yu, Stefan Dietze, and Ralph Ewerth. 2018. Current Challenges for Studying Search as Learning Processes. In Proceedings of Learning and Education with Web Data, Amsterdam, Netherlands, May 2018 (LILE2018), 4 pages. https://doi.org/10.1145/nnnnnnn.nnnnnnnGoogle ScholarGoogle Scholar
  6. Saskia Brand-Gruwel, Iwan Wopereis, and Amber Walraven. 2009. A descriptive model of information problem solving while using internet. Computers & Education, 53(4), 1207--1217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2009.06.004Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  7. John H. Flavell. 1979. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive--developmental inquiry. American psychologist, 34(10), 906--911. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  8. Thomas O. Nelson, and John Dunlosky. 1992. How shall we explain the delayed-judgment-of-learning effect? Psychological Science, 3(5), 317--319. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467--9280.1992.tb00681.xGoogle ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  9. Roger Azevedo. 2005. Computer environments as metacognitive tools for enhancing learning. Educational Psychologist, 193--197. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4004_1Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  10. Richard E. Mayer. 2014. Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, Second Edition. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139547369.005Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  11. Don A. Moore, and Paul J. Healy. 2008. The trouble with overconfidence. Psychological review, 115(2), 502--517. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033--295X.115.2.502Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  12. John Dunlosky, and Katherine A. Rawson. 2012. Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self evaluations undermine students' learning and retention. Learning and Instruction, 22(4), 271--280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.08.003Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  13. Jeri L. Little, and Mark A. McDaniel. 2015. Individual differences in category learning: Memorization versus rule abstraction. Memory & cognition, 43(2), 283--297. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0475--1Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  14. Rakefet Ackerman, and Morris Goldsmith. 2011. Metacognitive regulation of text learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 18--32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022086Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  15. Rakefet Ackerman, and Tirza Lauterman. 2012. Taking reading comprehension exams on screen or on paper? A metacognitive analysis of learning texts under time pressure. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1816--1828. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.04.023Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  16. Adam L. Alter, and Daniel M. Oppenheimer. 2009. Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and social psychology review, 13(3), 219--235. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309341564Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  17. Allison J. Jaeger, and Jennifer Wiley. 2014. Do illustrations help or harm metacomprehension accuracy? Learning and Instruction, 34, 58--73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.08.002Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  18. Alwine Lenzner, Wolfgang Schnotz, and Andreas Müller. 2013. The role of decorative pictures in learning. Instructional Science, 41(5), 811--831. http://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-012--9256-zGoogle ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  19. Michael Kardas, and Ed O'Brien. 2018. Easier seen than done: Merely watching others perform can foster an illusion of skill acquisition. Psychological science, Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617740646Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  20. David Dunning. 2011. The Dunning--Kruger effect: On being ignorant of one's own ignorance. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 44, pp. 247--296). Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0--12--385522-0.00005--6Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  21. Claudio Aqueveque. 2018. Ignorant experts and erudite novices: Exploring the Dunning-Kruger effect in wine consumers. Food Qual. Prefer. 65, (2018), 181--184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.12.007Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  22. Ian G. Anson. 2018. Partisanship, Political Knowledge, and the Dunning-Kruger Effect. Polit. Psychol. 39, 5 (2018), 1173--1192. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12490Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  23. Shirley Gibbs, Kevin Moore, Gary Steel, and Alan McKinnon. 2017. The Dunning-Kruger Effect in a workplace computing setting. Comput. Human Behav. 72, (2017), 589--595. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.084Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  24. Gordon Pennycook, Robert M. Ross, Derek J. Koehler, and Jonathan A. Fugelsang. 2017. Dunning--Kruger effects in reasoning: Theoretical implications of the failure to recognize incompetence. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 6 (December 2017), 1774--1784. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017--1242--7Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  25. Barbara Coombes. 2009. Generation Y: Are they really digital natives or more like digital refugees? Synergy 7, 1 (2009), 31--40.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  26. Shinichi Monoi, Nancy O'Hanlon, and Karen R. Diaz. 2005. Online searching skills: Development of an inventory to assess self-efficacy. J. Acad. Librariansh. 31, 2 (2005), 98--105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acalib.2004.12.005Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  27. Delroy Paulhus, and Patrick Dubois. (2014). Application of the Overclaiming Technique to Scholastic Assessment. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 74(6), 975--990. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164414536184Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  28. Stav Atir, Emily Rosenzweig, and David Dunning. 2015. When Knowledge Knows No Bounds: Self-Perceived Expertise Predicts Claims of Impossible Knowledge. Psychol. Sci. 26, 8 (2015), 1295--1303. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615588195Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  29. Stefan Engeser. 2005. Messung des expliziten Leistungsmotivs: Kurzform der Achievement Motives Scale. Retrieved 10.08.2010 from http://www.uni-trier.de/ fileadmin/fb1/prof/PSY/PGA/bilder/Engeser__2005__Kurzform_der_AMS.pdf.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  30. Shane Frederick. 2005. Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making. J. Econ. Perspect. 19, 4 (November 2005), 25--42. https://doi.org/10.1257/089533005775196732Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  31. Gerald Matthews, Sian Campbell, Shona Falconer, Lucy A. Joyner, Jane Huggins, Kirby Gilliland, . . . Joel S. Warm. 2002. Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: Task engagement, distress, and worry. Emotion, 2(4), 315--340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528--3542.2.4.315Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  32. Richard E. Mayer and Roxana Moreno. 1998. A split-attention effect in multimedia learning: Evidence for dual processing systems in working memory. Journal of Educational Psychology 90, 2: 312--320. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.90.2.312Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  33. Florian Schmidt-Weigand and Katharina Scheiter. 2011. The role of spatial descriptions in learning from multimedia. Computers in Human Behavior 27, 1: 22--28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.007Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  34. Lin-Miao Lin, Dewayne Moore, and Karen M. Zabrucky. 2001. An assessment of students' calibration of comprehension and calibration of performance using multiple measures. Reading Psychology, 22(2), 111--128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/027027101300213083Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  35. Priscilla Bell and David Volckmann. 2011. Knowledge surveys in general chemistry: Confidence, overconfidence, and performance. J. Chem. Educ. (2011). https://doi.org/10.1021/ed100328cGoogle ScholarGoogle Scholar
  36. Marian Krajc and Andreas Ortmann. 2008. Are the unskilled really that unaware? An alternative explanation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29, 724--738. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2007.12.006Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  37. Thomas Schlösser, David Dunning, Kerri L. Johnson, and Justin Kruger. 2013. How unaware are the unskilled? Empirical tests of the "signal extraction" counterexplanation for the Dunning-Kruger effect in self-evaluation of performance. J. Econ. Psychol. 39, (2013), 85--100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2013.07.004Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  38. Darwin P. Hunt. 2003. The concept of knowledge and how to measure it. Journal of Intellectual Capital, Vol. 4 No. 1, pp. 100--113. https://doi.org/10.1108/14691930310455414Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  39. Asher Koriat. 1997. Monitoring one's own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 349--370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096--3445.126.4.349Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  40. Christopher K. Hertzog, John Dunlosky, Ann E. Robinson, and Daniel Peter Kidder. 2003. Encoding fluency is a cue used for judgments about learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(1), 22--34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278--7393.29.1.22Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  41. Peter Gerjets, Yvonne Kammerer, and Benita Werner. 2011. Measuring spontaneous and instructed evaluation processes during Web search: Integrating concurrent thinking-aloud protocols and eye-tracking data. Learning and Instruction, 21(2), 220--231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.02.005Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  42. Robert A. Bjork, John Dunlosky, and Nate Kornell. 2013. Self-Regulated Learning: Beliefs, Techniques, and Illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64,417--444. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011--143823Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  43. Joachim Kimmerle, Johannes Moskaliuk, Aileen Oeberst, and Ulrike Cress. 2015. Learning and collective knowledge construction with social media: A process-oriented perspective. Educational Psychologist, 50(2), 120--137. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2015.1036273Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  44. Mathieu d'Aquin, Alessandro Adamou, Stefan Dietze, Besnik Fetahu, Ujwal Gadiraju, Ilire Hasani-Mavriqi, Peter Holtz, Joachim Kimmerle, Dominik Kowald, Elisabeth Lex, and Susana López-Sola. 2017 (September). AFEL: Towards Measuring Online Activities Contributions to Self-directed Learning. In ARTEL@ EC-TEL. http://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1997/paper5.pdfGoogle ScholarGoogle Scholar
  45. Mathieu d'Aquin, Dominik Kowald, Angela Fessl, Elisabeth Lex, and Stefan Thalmann. 2018, (April). Afel-analytics for everyday learning. In Companion of the The Web Conference 2018 on The Web Conference 2018 (pp. 439--440). International World Wide Web Conferences Steering Committee. https://doi.org/10.1145/3184558.3186206Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library

Index Terms

  1. Metacognitive Judgments in Searching as Learning (SAL) Tasks: Insights on (Mis-) Calibration, Multimedia Usage, and Confidence

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Sign in
    • Published in

      cover image ACM Conferences
      SALMM '19: Proceedings of the 1st International Workshop on Search as Learning with Multimedia Information
      October 2019
      25 pages
      ISBN:9781450369190
      DOI:10.1145/3347451
      • General Chairs:
      • Ralph Ewerth,
      • Stefan Dietze,
      • Anett Hoppe,
      • Ran Yu

      Copyright © 2019 ACM

      Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

      Publisher

      Association for Computing Machinery

      New York, NY, United States

      Publication History

      • Published: 15 October 2019

      Permissions

      Request permissions about this article.

      Request Permissions

      Check for updates

      Qualifiers

      • research-article

      Upcoming Conference

      MM '24
      MM '24: The 32nd ACM International Conference on Multimedia
      October 28 - November 1, 2024
      Melbourne , VIC , Australia

    PDF Format

    View or Download as a PDF file.

    PDF

    eReader

    View online with eReader.

    eReader