skip to main content
10.5555/2447556.2447678acmconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication PageshriConference Proceedingsconference-collections
research-article

Rhetorical robots: making robots more effective speakers using linguistic cues of expertise

Published:03 March 2013Publication History

ABSTRACT

Robots hold great promise as informational assistants such as museum guides, information booth attendants, concierges, shopkeepers, and more. In such positions, people will expect them to be experts on their area of specialty. Not only will robots need to be experts, but they will also need to communicate their expertise effectively in order to raise trust and compliance with the information that they provide. This paper draws upon literature in psychology and linguistics to examine cues in speech that would help robots not only to provide expert knowledge, but also to deliver this knowledge effectively. To test the effectiveness of these cues, we conducted an experiment in which participants created a plan to tour a fictional city based on suggestions by two robots. We manipulated the landmark descriptions along two dimensions of expertise: practical knowledge and rhetorical ability. We then measured which locations the participants chose to include in the tour based on their descriptions. Our results showed that participants were strongly influenced by both practical knowledge and rhetorical ability; they included more landmarks described using expert linguistic cues than those described using simple facts. Even when the overall level of practical knowledge was high, an increase in rhetorical ability resulted in significant improvements. These results have implications for the development of effective dialogue strategies for informational robots.

References

  1. E. Campione and J. Véronis. A large-scale multilingual study of silent pause duration. In Proc. Speech Prosody '02, pages 199--202, 2002.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  2. J. G. Carbonell. Metaphor: an inescapable phenomenon in natural language comprehension. Technical report, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, 1981.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  3. R. Carlson, K. Gustafson, and E. Strangert. Cues for hesitation in speech synthesis. In Proc. Interspeech '06, volume 6, 2006.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  4. M. Chi, R. Glaser, and E. Rees. Expertise in problem solving. Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, 1981.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  5. V. Chidambaram, Y.-H. Chiang, and B. Mutlu. Designing persuasive robots: how robots might persuade people using vocal and nonverbal cues. In Proc. HRI '12, HRI '12, pages 293--300, 2012. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  6. B. Fogg and H. Tseng. The elements of computer credibility. In Proc. CHI '99, pages 80--87, 1999. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  7. S. K. Foss and C. L. Griffin. Beyond persuasion: A proposal for an invitational rhetoric. Communications Monographs, 62(1):2--18, 1995.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  8. M. Glass, J. Kim, M. Evens, J. Michael, and A. Rovick. Novice vs. expert tutors: A comparison of style. In Proc. MAICS '99, pages 43--49, 1999.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  9. J. Ham, R. Bokhorst, R. Cuijpers, D. van der Pol, and J.-J. Cabibihan. Making robots persuasive: The influence of combining persuasive strategies (gazing and gestures) by a storytelling robot on its persuasive power. In Social Robotics, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, pages 71--83. Springer-Verlag, 2011. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  10. E. J. Hartelius. The rhetoric of expertise. PhD thesis, The University of Texas at Austin, 2008.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  11. N. Harvey and I. Fischer. Taking advice: Accepting help, improving judgment, and sharing responsibility. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70(2):117--133, 1997.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  12. J. R. Hobbs. Metaphor, metaphor schemata, and selective inferencing. Technical Report 204, SRI International, Menlo Park, CA, 1979.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  13. M. Hyde. The ethos of rhetoric. Univ South Carolina Press, 2004.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  14. E. Isaacs and H. Clark. References in conversation between experts and novices. J Experimental Psychology: General, 116(1):26, 1987.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  15. C. Kidd and C. Breazeal. Robots at home: Understanding long-term human-robot interaction. In Proc. IROS '08, pages 3230--3235, 2008.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  16. I. Kinchin. Effective teacher student dialogue: a model from biological education. J Biological Education, 37(3):110--113, 2003.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  17. J. Larkin, J. McDermott, D. Simon, and H. Simon. Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems. Science, 208(4450):1335, 1980.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  18. R. Looije, M. A. Neerincx, and F. Cnossen. Persuasive robotic assistant for health self-management of older adults: Design and evaluation of social behaviors. Intl J Human-Computer Studies, 68(6):386--397, 2010. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  19. G. Marwell and D. Schmitt. Dimensions of compliance-gaining behavior: An empirical analysis. Sociometry, pages 350--364, 1967.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  20. J. Noel. On the varieties of phronesis. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 31(3):273--289, 1999.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  21. M. Roubroeks, J. Ham, and C. Midden. The dominant robot: Threatening robots cause psychological reactance, especially when they have incongruent goals. Persuasive Technology, pages 174--184, 2010. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  22. M. Siegel, C. Breazeal, and M. Norton. Persuasive robotics: The influence of robot gender on human behavior. In Proc. IROS '09, pages 2563--2568, 2009. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  23. J. Sniezek and L. Van Swol. Trust, confidence, and expertise in a judge-advisor system. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 84(2):288--307, 2001.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  24. C. Torrey. How robots can help: Communication strategies that improve social outcomes. PhD thesis, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, 2009. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  25. C. Torrey, A. Powers, M. Marge, S. Fussell, and S. Kiesler. Effects of adaptive robot dialogue on information exchange and social relations. In Proc. HRI '06, pages 126--133, 2006. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library

Index Terms

  1. Rhetorical robots: making robots more effective speakers using linguistic cues of expertise

          Recommendations

          Comments

          Login options

          Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

          Sign in

          PDF Format

          View or Download as a PDF file.

          PDF

          eReader

          View online with eReader.

          eReader