skip to main content
10.1145/2077451.2077458acmconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication PagesapgvConference Proceedingsconference-collections
research-article

The influence of avatar (self and character) animations on distance estimation, object interaction and locomotion in immersive virtual environments

Published:27 August 2011Publication History

ABSTRACT

Humans have been shown to perceive and perform actions differently in immersive virtual environments (VEs) as compared to the real world. Immersive VEs often lack the presence of virtual characters; users are rarely presented with a representation of their own body and have little to no experience with other human avatars/characters. However, virtual characters and avatars are more often being used in immersive VEs. In a two-phase experiment, we investigated the impact of seeing an animated character or a self-avatar in a head-mounted display VE on task performance. In particular, we examined performance on three different behavioral tasks in the VE. In a learning phase, participants either saw a character animation or an animation of a cone. In the task performance phase, we varied whether participants saw a co-located animated self-avatar. Participants performed a distance estimation, an object interaction and a stepping stone locomotion task within the VE. We find no impact of a character animation or a self-avatar on distance estimates. We find that both the animation and the self-avatar influenced task performance which involved interaction with elements in the environment; the object interaction and the stepping stone tasks. Overall the participants performed the tasks faster and more accurately when they either had a self-avatar or saw a character animation. The results suggest that including character animations or self-avatars before or during task execution is beneficial to performance on some common interaction tasks within the VE. Finally, we see that in all cases (even without seeing a character or self-avatar animation) participants learned to perform the tasks more quickly and/or more accurately over time.

References

  1. Bard, C., Hay, L., and Fleury, M. 1985. Role of peripheral vision in the directional control of rapid aiming movements. Canadian Journal of Psychology 39, 151--161.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  2. Bodenheimer, B., Meng, J., Wu, H., Narasimham, G., Rump, B., McNamara, T. P., Carr, T. H., and Rieser, J. J. 2007. Distance estimation in virtual and real environments using bisection. ACM Applied Perception in Graphics and Visualization, 35--40. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  3. Di Luca, M. 2010. New method to measure end-to-end delay of virtual reality. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 19, 6, 569--584. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  4. Dodds, T., Mohler, B., and Blthoff, H. 2010. A communication task in hmd virtual environments: Speaker and listener movement improves communication. Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Conference on Computer Animation and Social Agents (CASA 2010 (6), 1--4.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  5. Durlach, N., and Slater, M. 2000. Presence in shared virtual environments and virtual togetherness. PRESENCE 9, 1, 214--217. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  6. Henry, D., and Furness, T. 1993. Spatial perception in virtual environments: Evaluating an architectural application. Virtual Reality Annual International Symposium, 33--40. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  7. Lok, B. C. 2004. Toward the merging of real and virtual spaces. Communications of the ACM 47, 8 (August), 21--46. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  8. Loomis, J., and Knapp, J. 2003. Visual perception of egocentric distances in real and virtual environments l. j. hettinger and m. w. haas, eds. erlbaum, mahwah, nj, ch. 2. Virtual and Adaptive Environments, 21--46.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  9. Mohler, B. J., Creem-Regehr, S. H., and Thompson, W. B. 2006. The influence of feedback on egocenteric distance judgments in real and virtual environments. In Proc. Symposium on Applied Perception in Graphics and Visualization, ACM, 9--14. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  10. Mohler, B., Campos, J., Weyel, M., and Blthoff., H. H. 2007. Gait parameters while walking in a head-mounted display virtual environment and the real world. Proceedings of the 13th Eurographics Symposium on Virtual Environments and 10th Immersive Projection Technology Workshop (IPT-EGVE 2007) (07), 85--88.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  11. Mohler, B. J., Blthoff, H., Thompson, W., and Creem-Regehr, S. 2008. A full-body avatar improves egocentric distance judgments in an immersive virtual environment. ACM Proceedings of the 5th Symposium on Applied Perception in Graphics and Visualization (APGV 08) (August), 194--197. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  12. Mohler, B., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Thompson, W., and Blthoff, H. H. 2010. The impact of an animated avatar on egocentric distance perception in an immersive virtual environment. European Conference on Visual Perception 39 ECVP Abstract Supplement (August), 51.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  13. Mohler, B. J., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Thompson, W. B., and Blthoff, H. H. 2010. The effect of viewing a self-avatar on distance judgments in an hmd-based virtual environment. PRESENCE (June). Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  14. Philbeck, J. W., Woods, A. J., Arthur, J., and Todd, J. 2008. Progressive locomotor recalibration during blind walking. Percept Psychophys 70, 8 (Nov), 14591470.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  15. Prinz, W. 1997. Perception and action planning. European journal of cognitive psychology.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  16. Proteau, L., Boivin, K., Linossier, S., and Abahnini, K. 2000. Exploring the limits of peripheral vision for the control of movements. Journal of Motor Behavior 32, 277--286.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  17. Reichenbach, A., Thielscher, A., Peer, A., Bülthoff, H., and Bresciani, J.-P. 2009. Seeing the hand while reaching speeds up on-line responses to a sudden change in target position. The Journal of Physiology 587, 19, 4605--4616.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  18. Richardson, A. R., and Waller, D. 2007. Interaction within immersive virtual environment corrects users distance estimates. Human Factors 49, 3, 507--517.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  19. Ries, B., Interrante, V., Kaeding, M., and Anderson, L. 2008. The effect of self-embodiment on distance perception in immersive virtual environments. In Proceedings of the ACM Symposium on Virtual Reality Software and Technology, 167--170. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  20. Ries, B., Interrante, V., Kaeding, M., and Phillips, L. 2009. Analyzing the effect of a virtual avatar's geometric and motion fidelity on ego-centric spatial perception in immersive virtual environments. In 16th ACM Symposium on Virtual Reality Software and Technology (VRST '09), ACM, New York, NY, USA, S. N. S. (Ed.), Ed., ACM, 59--66. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  21. Saunders, J., and Knill, D. 2004. Visual feedback control of hand movements. Journal of Neuroscience.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  22. Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., and Knoblich. 2006. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  23. Slater, M., and Usoh, M. 1994. Body centered interaction in immersive virtual environments. In N. M. Thalmann & D. Thalmann (Eds.), Artificial life and virtual reality. New York: John Wiley and Sons., 125148.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  24. Slater, M., Spanlang, B., Sanchez-Vives, M., and Blanke, O. 2006. First person experience of body transfer in virtual reality. PLoS ONE 5, 5: e10564.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  25. Slater, M. 2009. Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364, 1535 (Dec), 3549--3557.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  26. Spijkers, W., and Spellerberg, S. 1995. On-line control of rapid aiming movements? Acta Psychologica 90, 333--348.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  27. Streuber, S., and de la Rosa, S. 2010. The role of body and tool-based information in joint action coordination. European Conference on Visual Percpetion Conference Abstract.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  28. Streuber, S., de la Rosa, S., Trutoiu, L.-C. C., Blthoff, H. H., and Mohler, B. 2009. Does brief exposure to a self-avatar affect common human behaviors in immersive virtual environments? Eurographics 2009: The 30th Annual Conference of the European Association for Computer Graphics, 1--4. (Eds.) Stamminger, M., P. Dutr, Oxford, UK, Blackwell.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  29. Thompson, W. B., Willemsen, P., Gooch, A. A., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Loomis, J. M., and Beall, A. C. 2004. Does the quality of the computer graphics matter when judging distances in visually immersive environments? Presence: Tele-operators and Virtual Environments 13, 5, 560--571. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  30. Williams, B., Narasimham, G., Westerman, C., Rieser, J., and Bodenheimer, B. 2006. Functional similarities in spatial representations between real and virtual environments. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library

Index Terms

  1. The influence of avatar (self and character) animations on distance estimation, object interaction and locomotion in immersive virtual environments

        Recommendations

        Comments

        Login options

        Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

        Sign in
        • Published in

          cover image ACM Conferences
          APGV '11: Proceedings of the ACM SIGGRAPH Symposium on Applied Perception in Graphics and Visualization
          August 2011
          128 pages
          ISBN:9781450308892
          DOI:10.1145/2077451

          Copyright © 2011 ACM

          Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

          Publisher

          Association for Computing Machinery

          New York, NY, United States

          Publication History

          • Published: 27 August 2011

          Permissions

          Request permissions about this article.

          Request Permissions

          Check for updates

          Qualifiers

          • research-article

          Acceptance Rates

          APGV '11 Paper Acceptance Rate19of33submissions,58%Overall Acceptance Rate19of33submissions,58%

        PDF Format

        View or Download as a PDF file.

        PDF

        eReader

        View online with eReader.

        eReader